Review Article


DOI :10.26650/SP2021-1002572   IUP :10.26650/SP2021-1002572    Full Text (PDF)

Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement

Onur ÇalışkanAycan Kapucu

Emotion is structurally multifaceted with different components. One of these components, subjective experience, is pivotal in defining and measuring emotions. Self-report methods are used to ascertain the subjective experience of emotion. Unlike the neural and behavioral components of emotion, the qualities in subjective experience have been analyzed relatively less. However, for a more accurate and valid determination, the relevant structure should be well conceptualized. In this sense, the first part of this review precisely refers to the concept of emotional experience. This section concerns the distinctive meaning of subjective experience and details the correlation of subjective experience with other components of emotion. Next, the methods of accessing and expressing this experience will be discussed. Finally, the various scales in literature for measuring subjective experience are discussed. The status of these scales in Turkish and international literature is summarized. After conceptualizing subjective experience, the second part includes crucial aspects that may be useful for a more accurate and valid measurement. For this purpose, temporal issues that are important in measurement are addressed. First, the results of the temporal position of measurement, which may be in the past, future, or online; the results of the differences in the temporal emphasis in the instruction and the mechanism by which it affects the determination are mentioned. Subsequently, systematic explanations of individual differences in the experience of emotion are emphasized. Emotional granularity, referring to the extent to which an emotion is expressed in detail, and emotional dialecticism, indicating the state of experiencing different emotions together, are explained. Individual differences and their potential contribution to measuring and understanding emotion were addressed. The different views on the subjective experience of emotion, the temporal aspects, individual differences that can matter for measurement, and the mechanisms by which they can contribute to determination practices were discussed.

DOI :10.26650/SP2021-1002572   IUP :10.26650/SP2021-1002572    Full Text (PDF)

Duygu Ölçmede Kullanılan Öz Bildirim Yöntemleri: Neyi Ölçer? Nasıl Doğru Ölçer?

Onur ÇalışkanAycan Kapucu

Duygu farklı bileşenleri olan, çok yönlü bir yapıya sahiptir. Bu bileşenlerden biri olan öznel deneyim hem duygunun tanımında hem de ölçülmesinde önemli bir yer tutmaktadır. Duygunun öznel deneyiminin ölçümünde ise öz bildirim yöntemleri kullanılmaktadır. Fakat literatüre bakıldığında duygunun nöral ve davranışsal bileşenlerinin aksine öznel deneyimindeki niteliklerin görece daha az incelendiği görülmektedir. Oysa ölçümün daha doğru ve geçerli yapılabilmesi için ilgili yapının önce iyi kavramsallaştırılmış olması gerekmektedir. Bu anlamda derlemenin birinci kısmı duygu deneyiminin içeriğinin tam olarak neye tekabül ettiğine ilişkindir. Bu kısımda ilk olarak öznel deneyimin diğer duygu bileşenleriyle olan ilişkileri ve tek başına öznel deneyimin ne ifade ettiği detaylandırılmıştır. Sonrasında ise deneyime erişimin ve bu deneyimin ifadesinin nasıl mümkün olduğundan bahsedilmiştir. Son olarak öznel deneyimin ölçümünü için geliştirilmiş olan literatürde yaygın şekilde kullanılan farklı ölçekler üzerinde durulmuştur. Hem ulusal hem uluslararası literatürdeki ölçeklerin güncel durumları özetlenmiştir. Öznel deneyimin kavramsallaştırılmasının ardından ikinci kısım, daha doğru ve geçerli bir ölçüm için fayda sağlayabilecek önemli hususları içermektedir. Bu amaçla derlemenin ikinci kısmında öncelikle ölçümde dikkat edilmesi gereken zamansal hususlara yer verilmiştir. Önce ölçümün zamansal pozisyonunun geçmişte, gelecekte veya anlık olmasının sonuçlarına, sonrasında yönergede zamansal vurgudaki farkların sonuçlarına ve ölçümü nasıl etkilediğine dair çalışmalara değinilmiştir. Ardından duygunun deneyiminde yaşanan bireysel farklılıkların sistematik açıklamaları üzerinde durulmuştur. Duygunun ne ölçüde detaylı bir şekilde ifade edildiğine işaret eden duygusal detaycılık, farklı duyguların beraber yaşanma durumunu ifade eden duygusal diyalektisizm kavramları açıklanmıştır. Bu bireysel farklılıkların duygunun hem ölçümü hem de anlaşılmasına nasıl bir katkı sağlayabileceği ele alınmıştır. Tartışmada, duygunun öznel deneyimine dair farklı görüşler, zamansal hususların ve bireysel farklılıkların ölçüm için ne ifade edebileceği, ölçüm pratiklerinde nasıl katkılar sağlayabileceği tartışılmıştır.


EXTENDED ABSTRACT


Scientists define a neural loop, a neurobiological process, a phenomenological experience and feeling, and a perceptual-cognitive process (Izard, 2010). Emphasizing such different aspects of emotion leads to various techniques used to measure and evaluate these aspects in research.

Third-person techniques involve behavioral, neural, and physiological determination of emotion. These techniques provide objective data that are equally accessible. On the other hand, first-person methods are based on the subjective experiences of people. These methods are based on self-reports by the person experiencing the phenomenon. The selfreport method assesses the phenomenological aspect of emotion based on subjective experience.

Searle’s biological naturalism (Searle, 1992, 2000, 2004) provides a framework for scientifically examining emotional experience (Barrett et al., 2007). The first principle expounds on the reasons and distinct features that differentiate the content of emotional experiences from those of other experiences. The second principle emphasizes that the content of emotional experience cannot be entirely reduced to causes, as it is associated with specific brain systems. The third principle underscores that conscious experiences occur solely from an individual’s perspective and are inherently subjective, making them inaccessible to evaluation through third-person techniques. The purpose of science is to evaluate these subjective experiences in an epistemologically objective way (Barrett et al., 2007).

Understanding emotional experience requires distinguishing between the raw phenomenological and the reflective consciousness aspects (Nielsen & Kaszniak 2006). James (1890, 1983) characterized this distinction as the “feltness of a feeling” and its “perception by a subsequent reflective action,” while Lambie and Marcel (2002) frame it as “primary-order phenomenological experience” and “secondary-order awareness” (as cited in Nielsen & Kaszniak, 2006, p. 364). ” The raw phenomenological aspect deals with the part that defines the natural form of an emotion. Reflective consciousness enables the collection of essential information to form the experience in a cognitive layer that includes the environmental and situational information, thoughts, and evaluations at the time of the experience.

The dimensional approach to emotions focuses on two primary components: valence and arousal (Russell, 1980; Barrett & Russell, 1999). Valence determines the positive and negative qualities of an emotion by reflecting the pleasurable tone of an experience. This dimension is related to raw phenomenology (Barrett et al., 2007). Arousal represents the change in physiological activity; however, the extent to which this dimension corresponds to natural phenomenology is unclear, as cross-cultural studies have revealed that this dimension is represented in different languages (Barrett et al., 2007). Based on appraisal theories, emotions arise from evaluations, and subjective experience includes their reflection (Moors, 2020). The contents reflected in consciousness need not be labeled with a specific category of emotions. While emotional experience includes evaluative dimensions and a reflection of outcomes into consciousness, it can also be experienced without emotion category labeling.

Emotional experience has various neural and behavioral correlations. Using thirdperson methods for measuring experiences does not fully reflect the experience content of emotions since experience can exist independently of its other correlates. For example, in one study, Parkinson’s Disease patients with problems forming facial expressions reported experiences involving emotional valence and intensity (Reid, 2000). In addition, the content of the experience can only be accessed by the person concerned (Barrett et al., 2007). Because of this ontological reason, accessing this knowledge is possible via the self-reports of the experiencer.

Self-report methods in emotion-related research are grounded in two fundamental theoretical perspectives: the dimensional approach, exemplified by the circumplex model (Russell, 1980), and the discrete emotions perspective (Ekman, 1999; Izard, 2007; Panksepp, 2007). The former defines emotions along valence and arousal dimensions, as observed in measures like the Self-Assessment Manikin (Bradley & Lang, 1994) and Affect-Grid (Russell et al., 1998). The latter posits specific emotions as distinct categories, as reflected in instruments like the Differential Emotions Scale and the more recent Discrete Emotions Questionnaire (Harmon-Jones et al., 2016a).

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [PANAS] (Watson et al., 1988) is widely used for measuring emotions by assessing the intensity of positive and negative feelings through Likert scales. An updated version, PANAS-X (Watson & Clark, 1994), expands the evaluated expressions to include discrete emotions along with valence. Profile of Mood States [POMS] (McNair et al., 1992) and the Multiple Emotion Adjective Checklist [MAACL] (Zuckerman & Lubin, 1965) are scales that can be associated with the discrete emotion view and are primarily used in the field of psychiatry.

Different practices stand out during the measurement of emotional experience. In this direction, various research findings on the mechanisms by which the process of measuring temporal issues and individual differences can increase validity are discussed in the present article.

Temporal Issues

Robinson and Clore (2002) argue that in the accessibility model, in addition to experiential knowledge, beliefs about emotion also play a role in reporting emotions. Four different sources of information about emotional experience are used in self-reports: a) online assessments of feelings, b) episodic information, c) semantic information including situation-specific beliefs, and d) semantic information including identity-related beliefs (p. 937). According to the accessibility model, the source of information for online emotion reports is experiential emotions. Conversely, episodic information presents non-experiential information due to peak bias or recency effect. The model states that the missing details are complemented by semantic information if episodic information is inaccessible. In summary, biases in episodic memory and information from semantic memory cause different types of information to be reported rather than the original experiential information in retrospective reports.

Harmon-Jones and colleagues (2016b) investigated the effects of the change in temporal emphasis in the instructions given to the participants before the scale could make a difference in self-reports. In this study, the participants were asked what they felt at that moment or during the event that evoked an emotion, and it was found that the participants reported their emotions more intensely in the “during” condition than in the “right now” condition.

Individual Differences

Emotional granularity (Tugade et al., 2004) or emotional differentiation (Barrett et al., 2001) is the ability to characterize emotional experiences verbally. While someone with a high level of detail may prefer various representations with different qualities referring to the related emotion when reporting their experience (e.g., for fear: “shudder,” “terrified,” and “frightened”), someone with low detail may prefer more comprehensive and general expressions (e.g., for fear: “feeling so bad”).

Emotional dialecticism refers to the co-occurrence and temporal relations between positive and negative emotional experiences (Lindquist & Barrett, 2008). Differences in affective dialecticism are associated with the degree to which a culture indicates an interdependent or independent structure (Bagozzi et al., 1999). Resilience to stress (Rafaeli et al., 2007), cognitive complexity (Reich et al., 2001), age (Carstensen et al., 2000), and gender (Bagozzi et al., 1999) are also factors that impact dialecticism to varying extent.

Discussion

Subjective experiences, including neural and behavioral components, play a significant role in explaining emotions. While the neural and behavioral correlates of experience provide insights, they cannot capture the rich phenomenological content of emotions, as individuals experience these first-hand. Despite the epistemic advantage of direct access to mental experience through self-report methods, the literature suggests that third-person methods can also be employed to measure mental experience (Pauen & Haynes, 2021). The argument posits that third-person methods are currently in an early stage of development for this purpose; but their maturity in the future will be achieved through methods like triangulation (Pauen & Haynes, 2021).

In terms of measurement, two aspects of emotional experience need attention. First, considering existing standardized measurement methods, self-reporting appears to be the singular and optimal approach, which aligns with the perspective of Pauen and Haynes (2021). Second, Pauen and Haynes (2021) suggested that if third-person methods undergo sufficient development, they could enhance the accuracy and validity of the measurement, possibly providing better measurements than self-report methods. However, if the essence of the measurements concerns the “experience of emotion,” they would lack construct validity.

This highly precise measurement provides information other than the phenomenology of experiencing green color when observing a leaf. While spectroscopy accurately measures the biocomponent triggering the experience, it differs from quantitatively determining the wavelength of light reflected from the leaf. In this context, emotional experience is more nuanced because it is richer in content than color experience. Although third-person methods offer precise measurements, they will miss a significant part of the picture.

Measuring the time interval of the emotional experience seems to be the most accurate way to access “raw” information. Participants should be clearly instructed about the state of the range of emotional experiences measured in the study. Creating a framework for the scales in use or which need to be constructed based on the information provided by individual differences would contribute uniquely to their differences outside of their theoretical foundation.

A clear conceptualization of the subjective experience measured by these methods and an exploration of measurement procedures can be further improved, which is essential. A refined conceptualization of the subjective experience of emotion would also enhance the contribution of third-party methods to the measurement process. Future studies should focus on integrating the prevalent third-party techniques with self-report methods to improve the validity of emotional measurement.


PDF View

References

  • Adolphs, R. ve Anderson, D. J. (2018). The neuroscience of emotion: A new synthesis. Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.23943/9781400889914 google scholar
  • Afacan, Y. (2021). Impacts of biophilic design on the development of gerotranscendence and the Profile of Mood States during the COVID-19 pandemic. Ageing & Society, 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1017/ S0144686X21001860 google scholar
  • Arıkan İyilikci, E. ve Amado, S. (2018). The uncertainty appraisal enhances the prominent deck B effect in the Iowa gambling task. Motivation and Emotion, 42(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1007/ s11031-017-9643-5 google scholar
  • Aydın, A., Araz, A.ve Asan, A. (2011). Görsel Analog Ölçeği ve Duygu Kafesi: Kültürümüze uyarlama çalışması. Türk Psikoloji Yazıları, 14 (27), 1-13. google scholar
  • Bachorowski, J. A. ve Owren, M. J. (1995). Vocal expression of emotion: Acoustic properties of speech are associated with motional intensity and context. Psychological Science, 6(4), 219-224. https:// doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00596.x google scholar
  • Bagozzi, R. P., Wong, N. ve Yi, Y. (1999). The role of culture and gender in the relationship between positive and negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 13(6), 641-672. https:// doi:10.1080/026999399379023 google scholar
  • Barrett, L. F. (2006a). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1), 28-58. https://doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x google scholar
  • Barrett, L. F. (2006b). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 20-46. https://doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2 google scholar
  • Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C. ve Benvenuto, M. (2001). Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition & Emotion, 15(6), 713-724. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000239 google scholar
  • Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N. ve Gross, J. J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373-403. https://doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709 google scholar
  • Barrett, L. F. ve Russell, J. A. (1999). The structure of current affect: Controversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(1), 10-14. https://doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00003 google scholar
  • Bradley, M. M. ve Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49-59. https://doi:10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9 google scholar
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M. ve Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. M. Lewis ve J. M. Haviland-Jones (Ed.), Handbook of emotions içinde (2. Basım, s. 173-191). Guildford Press. google scholar
  • Campbell, D. T. ve Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 81-105. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046016 google scholar
  • Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange Theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. The American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106-124. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1415404 google scholar
  • Carroll, J. M. ve Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(2), 205-218. https:// doi:10.1037//0022-3514.70.2.205 google scholar
  • Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U. ve Nesselroade, J. R. (2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 644-655. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.644 google scholar
  • Carstensen, L. L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., Ram, N., Ersner-Hershfield, H., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Brooks, K. P. ve Nesselroade, J. R. (2011). Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 21-33. https://doi. org/10.1037/a0021285 google scholar
  • Christopoulos, G. I., Uy, M. A. ve Yap, W. J. (2019). The body and the brain: Measuring skin conductance responses to understand the emotional experience. Organizational Research Methods, 22(1), 394420. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428116681073 google scholar
  • Clark-Polner, E., Wager T. D., Satpute, A. B. ve Barrett, L. F. (2016). Neural fingerprinting: Meta-analysis, variation, and the search for brain-based essences in the science of emotion. Barrett L. F., Lewis M. ve Haviland-Jones J. M. (Ed.), The handbook of emotion içinde (4. basım, s. 146-165). The Guilford Press. google scholar
  • Coan, J. ve Allen, J. (2003). Varieties of emotional experience during voluntary emotional facial expressions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000(1), 375-379. https://doi. org/10.1196/annals.1280.034 google scholar
  • Cordaro, D. T., Sun, R., Keltner, D., Kamble, S., Huddar, N. ve McNeil, G. (2018). Universals and cultural variations in 22 emotional expressions across five cultures. Emotion, 18(1), 75-93. https:// doi.org/10.1037/emo0000302 google scholar
  • Desmet, P. (2003). Measuring emotion: Development and application of an instrument to measure emotional responses to products. Blythe, M.A., Overbeeke, K., Monk, A.F ve Wright, P.C. (Ed) Funology. Human-computer interaction series içinde (s. 111-123). Dordrecht. https://doi. org/10.1007/1-4020-2967-5_12 google scholar
  • Dinç, D. ve Aslan, A. (2017). Emotional and physiological reactions to classical Turkish music. Sobider, 4(16)., 294-304 http://dx.doi.org/10.16990/SOBIDER.3747 google scholar
  • Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. T. Dalgleish ve M. J. Power (Ed.), Handbook of cognition and emotion içinde (s. 45-60). Erişim adresi: https://www.paulekman.com/wp-content/uploads/2013/07/ Basic-Emotions.pdf google scholar
  • Ekman, P. ve Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system: Investigator’s guide. Consulting Psychologists Press. google scholar
  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V. ve Hager, J. C. (2002). Facial action coding system. Manual and investigator’s guide. Research Nexus. google scholar
  • Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W. ve Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221 (4616), 1208-1210. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6612338 google scholar
  • Er, N. (2006). Duygu durum sıfat çiftleri listesi. Psikoloji Çalışmaları, 26, 21-44 google scholar
  • Erol-Korkmaz, H. T. (2019). Örgütsel çatışmaya yönelik bir günlük çalışması: Çatışmanın olumsuz etkilerini artıran unsurlar. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 34(83), 20-37. https://doi.org/10.31828/ tpd1300443320180124m000002 google scholar
  • Feldman, L. A. (1995). Valence focus and arousal focus: Individual differences in the structure of affective experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 153-166. google scholar
  • Gardhouse, K. ve Anderson, A. K. (2013). Objective and subjective measurements in affective science. Armony J. ve Vuilleumier P. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience içinde (s. 57-81). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511843716.005 google scholar
  • Garfinkel, S. N., Minati, L., Gray, M. A., Seth, A. K., Dolan, R. J. ve Critchley, H. D. (2014). Fear from the heart: Sensitivity to fear stimuli depends on individual heartbeats. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(19), 6573-6582. https://doi.org/10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.3507-13.2014 google scholar
  • Gençöz, T. (2000). Pozitif ve Negatif Duygu Ölçeği: Geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 15(46), 19-26. google scholar
  • Gohm, C. L. ve Clore, G. L. (2000). Individual differences in emotional experience: Mapping available scales to processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 679-697. https:// doi:10.1177/0146167200268004 google scholar
  • Gray, E. K. ve Watson, D. (2007). Assessing positive and negative affect via self-report. Coan, J. A. ve. Allen, J. J. B (Ed.), Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment içinde (s. 171-183). Oxford University Press. google scholar
  • Gök, A. C., Selçuk, E. ve Gençöz, T. (2018). Olumlu ve olumsuz duygulanımın tekrarlanan ölçümlerde kişi-içi güvenirliği. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 33(82), 53-66. https://doi.org/10.31828/ tpd13004433.2018.8202.04 google scholar
  • Hamann, S. (2012). Mapping discrete and dimensional emotions onto the brain: Controversies and consensus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(9), 458-466. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.006 google scholar
  • Harmon-Jones, C., Bastian, B. ve Harmon-Jones, E. (2016a). The discrete emotions questionnaire: A new tool for measuring state self-reported emotions. PLoS ONE, 11(8), e0159915. https://doi. org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159915 google scholar
  • Harmon-Jones, C., Bastian, B. ve Harmon-Jones, E. (2016b). Detecting transient emotional responses with improved self-report measures and instructions. Emotion, 16(7), 1086-1096. https:// doi:10.1037/emo0000216 google scholar
  • Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 260-280. https://doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x google scholar
  • Izard, C. E. (2010). The many meanings/Aspects of emotion: Definitions, functions, activation, and regulation. Emotion Review, 2(4), 363-370. https://doi:10.1177/1754073910374661 google scholar
  • Izard, C. E., Libero, D. Z., Putnam, P. ve Haynes, O. M. (1993). Stability of emotion experiences and their relations to traits of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(5), 847860. https://doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.847 google scholar
  • James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9(34), 188-205. Erişim adresi: https://www.jstor.org/ stable/2246769?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents google scholar
  • Kapucu, A., Arıkan İyilikçi, E., Eroğlu, S. ve Amado, A. (2018). Korku ve kızgınlık duygularının nötr kelimelere ilişkin tanıma belleği performansına etkileri. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 33(82), 85-89. https://doi.org/10.31828/tpd.13004433.2018.82.02.06 google scholar
  • Kaszniak, A. W., Reminger, S. L., Rapcsak, S. Z. ve Glisky, E. L. (1999). Conscious experience and autonomic response to emotional stimuli following frontal lobe damage. S. R. Hameroff, A.W. Kaszniak ve D. J. Chalmers (Ed.), Toward a science of consciousness: III. The third Tucson discussions and debates içinde (s. 201-213). https://doi:10.1016/s0898-1221(00)90179-2 google scholar
  • Kuppens, P. ve Tong, E. M. W. (2010). An appraisal account of individual differences in emotional experience. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(12), 1138-1150. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1751-9004.2010.00324.x google scholar
  • LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4-5), 727-738. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1025048802629 google scholar
  • LeDoux, J. E. (2017). Semantics, surplus meaning, and the science of fear. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(5), 303-306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.004 google scholar
  • Lindquist, K. A. ve Barrett, L. F. (2008). Emotional complexity. M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones ve L. F. Barrett (Ed.), Handbook of emotions içinde (s. 513-530). The Guilford Press. google scholar
  • Matsumoto, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M. N., O’Sullivan, M. ve Frank, M. (2008). M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones ve L. F. Barrett (Ed.), Handbook of emotions içinde (s. 211-234). The Guilford Press. google scholar
  • Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H. ve Gross, J. J. (2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion, 5(2), 175-190. https:// doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.175 google scholar
  • Mauss, I. B. ve Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 209-237. https://doi:10.1080/02699930802204677 google scholar
  • McNair, D., Lorr, M. ve Droppleman, L. (1992). Revised manual for the profile of mood states. Educational and Industrial Testing Services. google scholar
  • Moors, A. (2014). Flavors of appraisal theories of emotion. Emotion Review, 6(4), 303-307. https://doi. org/10.1177/1754073914534477 google scholar
  • Moors, A. (2020). Appraisal theory of emotion. Encyclopedia ofpersonality and individual differences içinde (s. 232-240). Springer International Publishing. google scholar
  • Nielsen, L.A. ve Kaszniak, A.W. (2006). Conceptual, theoretical, and methodological ıssues in inferring subjective emotion experience recommendations for researchers. Coan, J. A. ve Allen, J. J. (Ed.) Series in affective science: Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment içinde (s. 361-375). Oxford University Press. google scholar
  • Panksepp, J. (2007). Neurologizing the psychology of affects: How appraisal-based constructivism and basic emotion theory can coexist. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 281-295. https:// doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00045.x google scholar
  • Pauen, M. ve Haynes, J. D. (2021). Measuring the mental. Consciousness and Cognition, 90, 103106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103106 google scholar
  • Peng, K. ve Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54(9), 741. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741 google scholar
  • Perusini, J. N. ve Fanselow, M. S. (2015). Neurobehavioral perspectives on the distinction between fear and anxiety. Learning & Memory, 22(9), 417-425. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.039180.115 google scholar
  • Quigley, K. S., Lindquist, K. A. ve Barrett, L. F. (2014). Inducing and measuring emotion and affect: Tips, tricks, and secrets. H. T. Reis ve C. M. Judd (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology içinde (s. 220-252). Cambridge University Press google scholar
  • Rafaeli, E., Rogers, G. M. ve Revelle, W. (2007). Affective synchrony: Individual differences in mixed emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(7), 915-932. https://doi. org/10.1177/0146167207301009 google scholar
  • Reich, J. W., Zautra, A. J. ve Potter, P. T. (2001). Cognitive structure and the independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 99-115. https://doi:10.1521/ jscp.20.1.99.22255 google scholar
  • Reid, S. A. (2000). The experience of emotion in Parkinson’s disease and normal aging: Assessing the roles of facial expressiveness and cognitive functioning. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, 60 (9), 4905. https://doi:10.4103/0976-3147.143197 google scholar
  • Robinson, M. D. ve Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 934-960. https://doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.934x google scholar
  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161-1178. https://doi:10.1037/h0077714 google scholar
  • Russell, J., Weiss, A. ve Mendelsohn, G. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493-502. https://doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.493 google scholar
  • Schimmack, U., Oishi, S. ve Diener, E. (2002). Cultural influences on the relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian dialectic philosophies or individualism-collectivism?. Cognition & Emotion, 16(6), 705-719. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.582 google scholar
  • Searle, J. R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. The MIT Press. google scholar
  • Searle, J. R. (2000). Consciousness. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 557-578. https://doi. org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.557 google scholar
  • Searle J. R. (2004). Mind: A brief introduction. Oxford University Press. google scholar
  • Selvi, Y., Güleç, M., Aydın, A. ve Beşiroğlu, L. (2011). Duygudurum Profili’nin (DP) Türkçe formunun psikometrik değerlendirmesi. Journal Of Mood Disorders, 1(4), 152-61. https://doi.org/10.5455/ jmood.20110902072033 google scholar
  • Selvi, Y., Kılıç, S., Aydın, A. ve Özdemir, P. G. (2015). The effects of sleep deprivation on dissociation and profiles of mood, and its association with biochemical changes. Nöro Psikiyatri Arşivi, 52(1), 83. https://doi.org/10.5152/npa.2015.7116 google scholar
  • Simon-Thomas, E. R., Keltner, D. J., Sauter, D., Sinicropi-Yao, L. ve Abramson, A. (2009). The voice conveys specific emotions: Evidence from vocal burst displays. Emotion, 9(6), 838-846. https://doi. org/10.1037/a0017810 google scholar
  • Solomon, R. L. ve Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81(2), 119-145. https://doi:10.1037/h0036128 google scholar
  • Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K. ve Wang, L. (2010). Dialecticism and the co-occurrence of positive and negative emotions across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41(1), 109-115. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0022022109349508 google scholar
  • Storbeck, J. ve Maswood, R. (2016). Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory capacity where sadness does not: Emotion, working memory and executive control. Cognition & Emotion, 30(5), 925-938. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1034091 google scholar
  • Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L. ve Barrett, L. F. (2004). Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. Journal ofPersonality, 72(6), 1161-1190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x google scholar
  • Vogel, E. K. ve Awh, E. (2008). How to exploit diversity for scientific gain: Using individual differences to constrain cognitive theory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 171-176. https:// doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00569.x google scholar
  • Wackermann J. (2014). The long is not just a sum of the shorts: on time experienced and other times. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 516. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00516 google scholar
  • Watson, D. ve Clark, L.A. (1994). The PANAS-X manual for the positive and negative affect schedule. Erişim adresi: https://doi.org/10.17077/48VT-M4T2 google scholar
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A. ve Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-1070. https://doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 google scholar
  • Watson, D. ve Vaidya, J. (2003). Mood measurement: Current status and future directions. Schinka J. A. ve Velicer W. F. (Ed.), Handbook ofpsychology: Research methods in psychology içinde (s. 351375). John Wiley & Sons Inc. google scholar
  • Wilson, T. D. ve Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 131-134. https://doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00355.x google scholar
  • Winkielman, P. ve Berridge, K. C. (2004). Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 120-123. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00288.x google scholar
  • Yılmaz, T. ve Bekaroğlu, E. (2020). Ayrık Duygular Ölçeğinin Türkçeye uyarlama, güvenirlik ve geçerlik çalışması. Turkish Studies-Social Sciences, 15(4), 2233-2244. http://doi.org/10.29228/ TurkishStudies.40502 google scholar
  • Yüvrük, E., Kapucu, A. ve Amado, S. (2020). The effects of emotion on working memory: Valence versus motivation. Acta Psychologica, 202, 102983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102983 google scholar
  • Zautra, A. J., Berkhof, J. ve Nicolson, N. A. (2002). Changes in affect interrelations as a function of stressful events. Cognition & Emotion, 16(2), 309-318. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000257 google scholar
  • Zuckerman, M. ve Lubin, B. (1965). Normative data for the Multiple Affect Adjective Check List. Psychological Reports, 16(2), 438. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1965.16.2.438 google scholar

Citations

Copy and paste a formatted citation or use one of the options to export in your chosen format


EXPORT



APA

Çalışkan, O., & Kapucu, A. (2023). Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement. Studies in Psychology, 43(3), 393-428. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572


AMA

Çalışkan O, Kapucu A. Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement. Studies in Psychology. 2023;43(3):393-428. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572


ABNT

Çalışkan, O.; Kapucu, A. Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement. Studies in Psychology, [Publisher Location], v. 43, n. 3, p. 393-428, 2023.


Chicago: Author-Date Style

Çalışkan, Onur, and Aycan Kapucu. 2023. “Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement.” Studies in Psychology 43, no. 3: 393-428. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572


Chicago: Humanities Style

Çalışkan, Onur, and Aycan Kapucu. Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement.” Studies in Psychology 43, no. 3 (Apr. 2024): 393-428. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572


Harvard: Australian Style

Çalışkan, O & Kapucu, A 2023, 'Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement', Studies in Psychology, vol. 43, no. 3, pp. 393-428, viewed 28 Apr. 2024, https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572


Harvard: Author-Date Style

Çalışkan, O. and Kapucu, A. (2023) ‘Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement’, Studies in Psychology, 43(3), pp. 393-428. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572 (28 Apr. 2024).


MLA

Çalışkan, Onur, and Aycan Kapucu. Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement.” Studies in Psychology, vol. 43, no. 3, 2023, pp. 393-428. [Database Container], https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572


Vancouver

Çalışkan O, Kapucu A. Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement. Studies in Psychology [Internet]. 28 Apr. 2024 [cited 28 Apr. 2024];43(3):393-428. Available from: https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572 doi: 10.26650/SP2021-1002572


ISNAD

Çalışkan, Onur - Kapucu, Aycan. Self-Report Methods for Assessing Emotions: Understanding Structure and Accuracy of Measurement”. Studies in Psychology 43/3 (Apr. 2024): 393-428. https://doi.org/10.26650/SP2021-1002572



TIMELINE


Submitted30.09.2021
Accepted01.08.2023
Published Online15.12.2023

LICENCE


Attribution-NonCommercial (CC BY-NC)

This license lets others remix, tweak, and build upon your work non-commercially, and although their new works must also acknowledge you and be non-commercial, they don’t have to license their derivative works on the same terms.


SHARE




Istanbul University Press aims to contribute to the dissemination of ever growing scientific knowledge through publication of high quality scientific journals and books in accordance with the international publishing standards and ethics. Istanbul University Press follows an open access, non-commercial, scholarly publishing.